Matt Spike

the life logistic


Project maintained by matspike Hosted on GitHub Pages — Theme by mattgraham

Week 1 Reading: Introduction to evolutionary theory

Reading Quiz

You should take the quiz after doing the reading (this page AND ch. 1-2 of Fitch), but before the lecture (the quiz will close 1 hour before). Doing the quiz will enable you to check that you understand key concepts I want you to know, but it’ll also help me figure out what bits of the reading you struggled with, and I can address those in the lecture.

The quiz is on TopHat. Instructions for signing up to Top Hat via EASE are here; the course join code is forthcoming When you have already signed up and joined the course:

Overview

The purpose of this week’s reading is to get you up to speed on the basics of evolutionary theory, which is no small task. The reading for this week, Chapters 1 and 2 (up to p. 72) of Fitch (2010) provides an excellent start (see the main course page for more on how to access the book, under Readings). This is important background to have for all students in the course, and especially so for the MSc students, who used to get much more depth about evolution in the much-missed (by me at least) Foundations of Evolution course - for this reason, I’ll be running the MSc tutorials myself to try and cover as much foundational evolutionary thought of this as possible under the circumstances.

As we’ll be using the Fitch text a fair amount, you should know a bit about its author: Tecumseh Fitch is a biologist and cognitive scientist based in Vienna, and has been a leading figure in evolutionary linguistics for the last 15 years or so. The book provides an excellent introduction of evolutionary linguistics complete with  a glossary you should use to make sense of unfamiliar terms. However, keep in mind the text is very much, erm, Fitch’s personal, not necessarily-shared-by-everyone perspective cough on the evolution of language (he did, after all, write it).

The field of evolutionary linguistics is relatively young, as we discussed in the first lecture. This has two consequences which you should keep in mind as we use Fitch’s book as our main lens for looking at language evolution.

The first is that a young field is likely to be less cohesive, and since the evolution language is inherently interdisciplinary, those who study it are a diverse bunch with diverse perspectives. Moreover, given the fact that many of these researchers are linguists - an academic community who, you may have noticed, are not blessed with a reputation for measured, temperate debate or a distaste for bitter ad-hominem disputes - there’s a healthy amount of disagreement on, well, pretty much any topic you can think of. This being the case, Fitch argues for positions which many (myself included) would disagree with – but, there is a broad consensus (or at least a broad consensus that there is a growing consensus) that the kind of evidence-based approach Fitch uses to ask evolutionary questions is the best way to move forward.

The second consequence is that the field can move fast, so some of our understanding has changed since this book was published. For these two chapters at least, everything pretty much holds, but when we get to the human evolutionary tree, for example, our understanding is rapidly changing. Not long after the ink dried on the first copies of Fitch’s book, a large team at the MPI published the Denisovangenome (more on this later). I’ll highlight places where there is disagreement or recent progress in the field as we progress through the course.

This week

Chapter 1 provides a brief outline of what it means to study “the evolution of language”, and highlights some of the major topics and debates in the field. Chapter 2 provides a very brief introduction to the basics of evolutionary biology (natural selection, and also a little bit on genetics, molecular biology, development) – the introduction to the basic ideas is extremely clear and readable, but much of this chapter focusses on debates in evolutionary biology that might be a bit obscure if you only just found out what natural selection is. The full chapter is worth reading anyway, and the latter sections on adaptation and constraints on adaptation are very useful, but to help you out I have provided some additional notes below on the basics of adaptation and natural selection, on the comparative method, and on cultural evolution. These should help you to digest chapter 2, particularly section 2.3 onward, so this is best to read ahead of time (hence the term pre-reading).

In addition to the details, I think there are several important themes that Fitch touches on that will crop up later in the course, so watch out for these:

Adaptation and natural selection

This is intended as a very simplified summary of what natural selection is – there are also literally dozens of textbooks or web tutorials you could consult. This should form a useful background to understanding Fitch’s chapter 2 (and the rest of the course!). FYI, I am working with a textbook called Evolution (they are all called Evolution), by Barton et al – see below for reference.

Adaptation

Everywhere you look in the natural world, you see animals which seem to be exquisitely well-designed for the world they live in: think of the superb camouflage systems of animals like stick-insects or chameleons, or the beautiful fit between flowers and their pollinators, of the ability of animals like camels to or penguins to survive in extreme heat or extreme cold,or the complex construction skills and social systems of ants and termites. These cases of apparent design are known as adaptations or adaptive traits (the process by which adaptations are formed is also known as adaptation by the way – adaptation produces adaptations).

How can we explain adaptations, the appearance of design in life? Pre-Darwininan explanations were either theological, or appealed to the idea that organisms changed to fit their environment and somehow passed hose changes on to their offspring (e.g. the idea was that if you lifted weights, your kids would inherit some of your acquired strength). These early theories have been replaced by one of the most important scientific ideas in human history: evolution by natural selection. All adaptations are the product of natural selection, and only natural selection leads to adaptation.

Evolution by natural selection

In this next section I am going to explain what natural selection is, and the preconditions for natural selection to occur. The concepts should be relatively straightforward, but if you don’t know anything about natural selection take a look at this very simple explanation with nice pictures, then read what follows – they all have the same simple components (variation, variation makes a difference in reproduction, variants are passed from parents to offspring). You could also watch a documentary – the BBC documentary Charles Darwin and the Tree of Life (60 minutes) is a great example.

Natural selection is the process by which genotypes with higher fitness increase in frequency in a population, and it arises whenever you have heritable variation in fitness (Barton, 2007). I’ll explain what that means, and why it leads to adaptation.

The genotype of an individual is that individual’s genetic make-up – the genetic information that determines, through complex interactions with the environment, an individual’s phenotype: their body, behaviours and other observable characteristics. Genetics and the processes by which genes influence bodies and behaviours are incredibly complex, and we don’t need to know about them for the purposes of this course, or indeed for understanding how natural selection works (indeed, Darwin knew nothing about genes). The crucial thing is that genes are passed from parent(s) to offspring, via asexual or sexual reproduction. If an aspect of an organism’s phenotype (say something about its physical appearance, or the behaviours it performs) is determined or influenced by that organism’s genes, then that means that that trait is heritable: it can be passed from parent to child. More informally, for heritable traits the offspring will resemble the parent. For instance, you probably physically resemble your parents – that’s because many aspects of your physical appearance are strongly influenced by your genes, and therefore are heritable. Heritability is one of the requirements for natural selection: traits that can’t be passed from parent to offspring can’t evolve by natural selection.

The second precondition for natural selection is that the population exhibits variation. This is obviously true at the level of phenotypes – you’ve probably noticed that people vary in their physical appearance, tastes, abilities and personality, and you can similarly observe variation within any species of animals, insects, or plants (although you might need to be relatively expert to spot it). And at least some of this variation is underpinned by variation at the genetic level: different individuals have different genotypes, and variation in their genes contributes to variation in their bodies and behaviours. This is therefore heritable variation, and heritable variation is required for natural selection to operate: if everybody ‘looked the same’ (i.e. had the same phenotype), or if the phenotypic variation that existed was not heritable, natural selection would not occur.

The final requirement for natural selection is that variation matters. In particular, it must matter for fitness, which is a technical term referring to the number of offspring an individual has: in order for natural selection to occur, variants must differ in fitness. While you may not have noticed any particularly systematic correspondences between phenotypic variants and fitness in your personal experience, these have been painstakingly explored and mapped out by biologists using observational and experimental techniques (these examples from Barton et al., chapter 19). For instance, biologists might carefully note the size of birds and their probability of survival (it turns out that, at least for some bird species, being too big or too little makes you more likely to die young, and survival is of course a prerequisite for having offspring; somewhat surprisingly, the same link between size and mortality appears to apply in human infants). It’s not hard to imagine that other heritable traits (i.e physical appearance, physical characteristics, abilities or behaviours) also impact on fitness, the number of offspring you have – therefore, if there is variation in those traits, we will have heritable variation in fitness: individuals inherit their traits from their parents, different individuals inherit different traits, and some of those traits are ‘better’ (in the sense of leading to more offspring) than others.

If all of these three components are in place, then natural selection ensues. Organisms with high-fitness traits will produce more copies of themselves than organisms with traits with lower fitness (by definition – remember, “high fitness” means “has lots of offspring”, “low fitness” means “has few offspring” ), and over time the population will come to be dominated by traits which are associated with high fitness.  Or, as the definition at the start of this section said, “Natural selection is the process by which genotypes with higher fitness increase in frequency in a population”, and this occurs whenever we have heritable variation in fitness.

Why does natural selection lead to adaptations? My informal definition of an adaptation was that it is a feature of an organism that appears to be well-designed for that organism’s environment. A better, more technical definition would be that an adaptation is a trait which functions to increase fitness, and that evolved for that function. Hopefully the link should now make sense: adaptations are traits that make organisms well-designed, i.e. that enable organisms to better survive and reproduce (both of which influence that organism’s fitness), and natural selection leads to the accumulation of those sorts of traits in organisms. Since natural selection has been operating for as long as we have had heritable variation in fitness on the planet (which is probably as long as life has existed), we are surrounded by organisms which are the product of many many many millions of years of natural selection – they have been exquisitely finely-tuned by natural selection.

The comparative method

Fitch advocates the comparative method: comparing the behaviours and abilities of multiple species in order to understand the evolution of human language. If you have studied any historical linguistics then the comparative method in biology will be pretty familiar to you – linguists have been comparing across languages to figure out what ancestral languages look like, and how languages change, for 200+ years. But if you are unfamiliar with the comparative method from linguistics, you might be wondering: why would studying other apes, monkeys or even whales or birds be useful to understanding human evolution?

All life on earth shares a common ancestor: if you trace your parents’ parents’ parents’ parents’ parents’ … back far enough (over 3 billion years) you’ll come to an organism (a very simple, single-cell affair) who is your ancestor, but also the ancestor of all other humans, all other primates, all other mammals, in fact of all living things on the planet (including all plants, all bacteria and so on). Similarly, if you go back less far in time you can find the last common ancestor of all animals, or all mammals, or all humans (the person who is the great great great great … grandparent of all living humans). This is because evolution produces a branching tree of life: populations split, become separated, and eventually accumulate so many changes (including adaptations) that they becomes separate species; those daughter species die out or in turn split into further daughter species, which die off or further split, and so on.

Humans are just one leaf of this vast and bushy tree of life, which means that we share varying degrees of relatedness to all the other living things on the planet: the species most closely related to us are now extinct (you can see some of them here, and we’ll talk about them later in the course), but our closest living relatives are the other hominids: chimps, gorillas, and orangutans. For example, our last common ancestor with chimpanzees (the individual who was a great great great … grandparent of all humans and all chimpanzees) lived around 6 or 7 million years ago. We are more distantly related to other mammals, other animals, and ultimately (by tracing our way back to our common ancestor) all other living things.

You will probably have noticed many similarities between humans and other hominids (most obviously chimps) – for starters, they look a lot like us, and many of their social behaviours are reminiscent of the way we behave. This is because we share many of their genes, passed on from parent to child all the way back to our shared ancestral population. Traits shared between species through common ancestry are known as homologies, and there are homologous traits between even distantly related species – for example, if you know where to look there are fairly obvious homologies in the structure of forelimbs in humans, dogs, whales and birds, in the way in which development works in related species, or in the genes encoding proteins like haemoglobin.

This is useful if you are trying to reconstruct the evolutionary history of a trait or ability, because by comparing across closely-related species you can make guesses about what the common ancestor of those species looked like, and how the daughter species diverged. For example, the similarities between humans and other apes (in the shape of our bodies, our social and communicative behaviours) presumably reflect our shared ancestry: the simplest explanation for these shared traits is that we all inherited them from our common ancestral species. Differences between us therefore must have developed after this point of common ancestry, in response to different selective pressures acting on our species since we separated and occupied different environments and different ecological niches.

Investigating the cognitive capacities of other apes (as we will spend some time doing in this course) therefore provides insights into the evolutionary history of our own species, because it allows us to infer what our ancestral species looked like, how we have changed since then, and what selective pressures might have driven those changes. For instance, in this course we are interested in the evolution of human language: to understand the evolutionary history of language in our species, we’ll need to try to figure out what kind of communication systems our ancestral species had (was it a lot like language, or a lot different?), and what selective pressures drove the evolution of the capacity for language in our species but not in closely related species (what were the survival and reproductive pressures made us evolve in the unique way we did?).

The more closely related two species are, the more homologous traits they will share. But studying distantly-related species can also be useful, because of convergent evolution, the process by which distantly-related species find similar solutions to similar survival or reproductive challenges. Consider the wings of birds and bats. They look similar (at least superficially) and serve the same function (enabling flight), but we know that this similarity is extremely unlikely to be due to descent from a common ancestor (bats and birds are only distantly related, and most of the other species that share the same common ancestor don’t have wings, which makes it unlikely that the common ancestor of bats and birds did). This means that flight must have independently evolved in bats and birds – natural selection discovered the same solution to the same set of environmental challenges (how to avoid predators and/or exploit hard-to-reach food sources) in two separate groups of animals. This is convergent evolution, and it produces analogies – traits that are similar not due to commondescent, but to shared selection pressures.  Studying analogous traitsprovides insights into the selective pressures that lead to theevolution of a particular behaviour or ability. For instance, in the case of language, if we could find an analogous trait in another species, we could try to figure out (by comparing these two species) what environmental or social challenges they share that might have lead to convergent evolution of the language-relevant ability in both species.

Cultural evolution

One of the crucial preconditions for evolution by natural selection is heredity: parents must pass their traits on to their offspring in order for those traits to be subject to natural selection. In the sections above I focussed exclusively on heredity that was driven by sexual (or asexual) reproduction: you inherit your genes from your parents, and those genes contribute via some very complex processes to determining what your phenotype looks like and the properties it has.

Genetics of course isn’t the only reason why offspring might resemble their parents: many of the things you know, the skills you have, and the beliefs that you hold were also inherited from your parents via learning. This might have involved explicit teaching (e.g. painstaking lessons on appropriate table manners), but much more of it will have involved much more subtle observational learning or social learning, where you figured out how to do stuff based on watching your parents in action. And, unlike sexual reproduction, social learning allows you to inherit knowledge, skills and behaviour from people other than your parents: your siblings, your friends, even your own biological offspring (everything I know about Minecraft I learnt from my kids).

The catch-all term for behaviours which are passed from individual to individual via learning, imitation, teaching etc is culture. Note that this is a technical usage of the term, which is related to but not the same as everyday uses in phrases like “high culture” (e.g. the opera) or “lad culture” (very much not the opera). Culture is clearly a massively important inheritance system for humans – art, religion, political beliefs, technology, and our huge and varied suite of skills for surviving in the world (think of anything from hunting antelope to crossing a road to programming a computer)  are all culturally transmitted, all aspects of human culture. But we’re not the only species who have culture: in fact, because the ability to learn from others is such a handy skill, many species  do social learning, including chimpanzees, crows, song birds, killer whales, rats and fish.

What’s this got to do with language? Firstly, language is culturally transmitted – you learn the language of your speech community based on exposure to their linguistic behaviour, which means it meets the standard definition of a culturally-transmitted behaviour (it’s a behaviour that’s passed from person to person via social learning). There are huge debates in the linguistics literature about how exactly language learning works – is it the unconstrained application of general-purpose learning procedures, or does linguistic input simply trigger latent structures that are prefigured in language learners’ brains? – but nobody doubts the central point that you need linguistic input from other people to end up with a language. Therefore, language is culturally transmitted.

What’s this got to do with evolution, and what does “cultural evolution” mean anyway? Firstly, there are clearly really interesting evolutionary questions about how the capacity for culture evolves. Two paragraphs ago I said “because the ability to learn from others is such a handy skill, many species do social learning” – in other words, I was claiming that the ability to do social learning is an adaptation (an ability that increases fitness), and therefore the ability to learn from others must be a product of natural selection.

But secondly, knowledge, behaviours and skills which are culturally transmitted evolve: they change over time. This is really obvious if you look at how technologies change: they are constantly improving, becoming more powerful, more functional, cheaper to manufacture, and generally better. But it’s also the case for other aspects of culture, including language. Languages change all the time – we wouldn’t want to say that the change from e.g. Old English to present-day English represents an improvement in the same way that present-day technologies are better than those in circulation 1000 years ago, but the fact that the language has changed as it’s been passed from generation to generation is undeniable – this is the process that Fitch refers to as glossogeny. In short, culture evolves. We’ll spend a good chunk of this course talking about cultural evolution, what it does to languages, and how the human capacity for culture might have evolved.

References

Barton, N. H., et al. (2007). Evolution. Cold Spring Harbor, NY: ColdSpring Harbor Laboratory Press.

Fitch, W. T. (2010). The Evolution of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ridley, M. (1996). Evolution. Oxford: Blackwell.